THE IMPACT OF INSTRUCTIONAL SUPPORT ON STUDENTS’ PERFORMANCE: AN EYE TRACKING STUDY
Keywords:
incomplete worked-out examples, completion problems, eye tracking, attention allocation, fixation duration.Abstract
This study examined the effects of various types of instructional support (incomplete worked-out examples - IWE; completion problems - CMP and conventional problems - CVP) on students’ attention allocation and performance as they use a computer-based learning environment. Eye movement and performance data were obtained from sixty-three university students who were randomly assigned to one of the three conditions. Results indicated significant differences regarding the number and duration of fixations, not only between the three conditions, but also between these conditions across the five genetics problems students solved during training. These findings indicate that the type of instructional support differently impacted the students’ allocation of visual attention, with participants in the IWE condition having significantly lower number of fixations and shorter fixation durations. In addition, the number and duration of fixations were significantly lower in the CMP condition than in the CVP condition, but only at the beginning and the end of training (i. e., problems 1 and 5). Finally, the amount of processing time as revealed by the eye tracking data was associated with the learning outcomes (e. g., training performance), indicating that visual attention is indeed related to the strategic processing during problem solving. The combined use of eye tracking data with learning measures provides a more comprehensive picture of the cognitive processes that underlie the problem solving.
ZUSAMMENFASSUNG. Diese Studie untersuchte die Effekte verschiedener Lernunterstützungen (ausgearbeitete Lösungsbeispiele – IWE; teilweise ausgearbeitete Problemlösungsaufgaben – CMP und konventionelle Problemlösungsaufgaben – CVP) auf die Aufmerksamkeit und Leistung während des Lernens in einer computergestützten Lernumgebung. Augenbewegungen und Leistungsdaten wurden von 63 Studierenden erhoben, die zufällig zu den drei Versuchsbedingungen zugeteilt wurden. Die Ergebnisse zeigten signifikante Unterschiede hinsichtlich der Zahl und Dauer der Fixierungen, nicht nur zwischen den drei Gruppen, sondern auch zwischen den Gruppen über die fünf Genetik-Aufgaben hinweg, die die Studierenden während des Trainings lösen mussten. Diese Ergebnisse zeigen, dass die verschiedenen Lernunterstützungen die visuelle Aufmerksamkeit der Studierenden unterschiedlich beeinflussten. Darüber hinaus korrelierte die Verarbeitungszeit, die durch die Augenbewegungen erfasst wurde, mit den Lernergebnissen (der Trainingsleistung), d. h. visuelle Aufmerksamkeit ist mit der strategischen Verarbeitung während des Problemlösens verknüpft. Die Verbindung von Eye-Tracking und Leistungsmessung ergibt ein umfassenderes Bild der kognitiven Prozesse, die dem Problemlösen zu Grunde liegen.
Schlüsselwörter: ausgearbeitete Lösungsbeispiele, teilweise ausgearbeitete Problemlösungsaufgaben, Eye-Tracking, Aufmerksamkeit, Fixationsdauer.
References
Atkinson, R.K., Derry, S.J., Renkl, A. & Wortham, D.W. (2000). Learning from examples: instructional principles from the worked examples research. Review of Educational Research, 70 (2), 181-214. DOI: 10.3102/00346543070002181
Brünken, R., Plass, J.L. & Leutner, D. (2003). Direct measurement of cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist, 38 (1), 53–61. DOI: 10.1207/S15326985EP3801_7
Catrambone, R. (1995). Aiding subgoal learning: Effects on transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87 (1),5-17. DOI: 10.037/0022-0663.87.1.5
Catrambone, R. (1996). Generalizing solution procedures learned from examples. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22 (4), 1020–1031. DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.22.4.1020
Chuang, H.-H. & Liu, H.-C. (2012). Effects of different multimedia presentations on viewers’ information-processing activities measured by eye-tracking technology. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 21, 276-286. DOI: 10.1007/s10956-011-9316-1
Cohen, J. (1992). A Power Primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112 (1), 155-159. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155
De Bruin, A. & Van Gog, T. (2012). Improving self-monitoring and self-regulation of learning: From cognitive psychology to the classroom. Learning and Instruction, 22, 245-298
Field, A. (2005). Discovering statistics using SPSS (2nd ed.). London: Sage
Gegenfurtner, A., Lehtinen, E. & Säljö, R. (2011). Expertise differences in the comprehension of visualizations: A meta-analysis of eye-tracking research in professional domains. Educational Psychology Review, 23(4), 523-552. DOI 10.1007/s10648-011-9174-7
Graesser, A.C., Lu, S., Olde, B.A., Cooper-Pye, E. & Whitten, S. (2005). Question asking and eye tracking during cognitive disequilibrium: Comprehending illustrated texts on devices when the devices break down. Memory & Cognition, 33(7), 1235-1247
Hegarty, M. & Just, M.A. (1993). Constructing mental models of machines from text and diagrams. Journal of memory and language, 32(6), 717-742
Hegarty, M., Mayer, R.E. & Green, C.E. (1992). Comprehension of arithmetic word problems: Evidence from students' eye fixations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(1), 76
Hegarty, M., Mayer, R.E. & Monk, C.A. (1995). Comprehension of arithmetic word problems: A comparison of successful and unsuccessful problem solvers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(1), 18
Just, M.A. & Carpenter, P.A. (1980). A theory of reading: from eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87(4), 329
Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19 (4), 509–539. DOI: 10.1007/s10648-007-9054-3
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P. & Sweller, J. (1999). Managing split-attention and redundancy in multimedia instruction. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13 (4), 351–371. DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199908)13:4<351::AID-ACP589>3.0.CO;2-6
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Tuovinen, J. & Sweller, J. (2001). When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. Journal of Education & Psychology, 93 (3), 579-588. DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.3.579
Knoblich, G., Öllinger, M. & Spivey, M. J. (2005). Tracking the eyes to obtain insight into insight problem solving. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Cognitive Processes in Eye Guidance (pp. 355-75).Oxford University Press, USA
Mihalca, L., Mengelkamp, C., Schnotz, W. & Paas, F. (2015). Completion problems can reduce the illusions of understanding in a computer-based learning environment on genetics. Contemporary Educational Psychology, DOI: http://dx.doi.org/DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.01.001
Paas, F., Tuovinen, J.E., Tabbers, H. & Van Gerven, P.W.M. (2003). Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory. Educational Psychologist 38 (1), 63–71. DOI: 10.1207/S15326985EP3801_8
Paas, F. & Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem solving skills: a cognitive load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86 (1), 122–133. DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.86.1.122
Paas, F. & Van Gog, T. (2006). Optimising worked example instruction: different ways to increase germane cognitive load. Learning and Instruction, 16 (2), 87–91. DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.02.004
Peters, M. (2010). Parsing Mathematical Constructs: Results from a Preliminary Eye Tracking Study. In M. Joubert (Ed.), Proceedings of the British Society for Research into Learning Mathematics, 30(2). British Society for Research into Learning Mathematics
Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372
Renkl, A. (2002). Worked-out examples: instructional explanations support learning by self-explanations. Learning and Instruction, 12 (5), 529-556. DOI: 10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00030-5
Renkl, A. & Atkinson, R.K. (2003). Structuring the transition from example study to problem solving in cognitive skill acquisition: A cognitive load perspective. Educational Psychologist, 38 (1), 15-22. DOI: 10.1207/S15326985EP3801_3
Renkl, A., Hilbert, T. & Schworm, S. (2009). Example-based learning in heuristic domains: A cognitive load theory account. Educational Psychological Review, 21, 67-78. DOI: 10.1007/s10648-008-9093-4
Schnotz, W. (2010). Reanalyzing the expertise reversal effect. Instructional Science, 38 (3), 315-323. DOI: 10.1007/s11251-009-9104-y
Schwonke, R., Berthold, K. & Renkl, A. (2009). How multiple external representations are used and how they can be made more useful. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 1227-1243
Stark, R. (1999). Lernen mit Lösungsbeispielen. Der Einfluß unvollständiger Lösungsschritte auf Beispielelaboration, Motivation und Lernerfolg [Learning by worked-out examples. The impact of incomplete solution steps on example elaboration, motivation, and learning outcomes]. Bern, Germany: Huber
Sweller, J., Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10(3), 251-296. DOI: 10.1023/A:1022193728205
Tai, R.H., Loehr, J.F. & Brigham, F.J. (2006). An exploration of the use of eye‐gaze tracking to study problem‐solving on standardized science assessments. International Journal of Research & Method in Education, 29(2), 185-208
Tsai, M.J., Hou, H.T., Lai, M.L., Liu, W.Y. & Yang, F.Y. (2012). Visual attention for solving multiple-choice science problem: An eye-tracking analysis. Computers & Education, 58(1), 375-385
Van Gog, T., Kester, L. & Paas, F. (2011). Effects of worked examples, example-problem, and problem-example pairs on novices’ learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36 (3), 212-218. DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.004
Van Gog, T., Kester, L., Nievelstein, F., Giesbers, B. & Paas, F. (2009). Uncovering cognitive processes: Different techniques that can contribute to cognitive load research and instruction. Computers in Human Behavior, 25(2), 325-331.
Van Gog, T., Paas, F. & Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2004). Process-oriented worked examples: Improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding. Instructional Science, 32, 83-98. DOI: 10.1023/B:TRUC.0000021810.70784.b0
Van Gog, T., Paas, F. & Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2008). Effects of studying sequences of process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer efficiency. Learning and Instruction, 18 (3), 211-222. DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.03.003
Van Gog, T., Paas, F., Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. & Witte, P. (2005). Uncovering the problem solving process: Cued retrospective reporting versus concurrent and retrospective reporting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 11, 237–244
Van Gog, T. & Rummel, N. (2010). Example-based learning: Integrating cognitive and social-cognitive research perspectives. Educational Psychological Review, 22 (2), 155-174. DOI: 10.1007/s10648-010-9134-7
Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. & de Croock, M.B.M. (1992). Strategies for computer-based programming instruction: Program completion vs. program generation. Journal of Educational Computing Research,8, 365-394. DOI: 10.2190/MJDX-9PP4-KFMT-09PM
Van Merriënboer, J.J.G., Schuurman, J.G., de Croock, M.B.M. & Paas, F. (2002). Redirecting learners' attention during training: Effects on cognitive load, transfer test performance and training. Learning and Instruction, 12, 11–37. DOI: 10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00020-2
Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: recent developments and future directions. Educational Psychology Review, 17 (2), 147-177. DOI: 10.1007/s10648-005-3951-0
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Copyright (c) 2015 Studia Universitatis Babeș-Bolyai Psychologia-Paedagogia
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.